I became a parent a year and a half ago, and my life changed
forever. When I was pregnant lots of parents gave me advice (Enjoy going to the
grocery store by yourself while you still can! Go out on dates! Clean your
house!). One even warned me that becoming a parent would “rock my world.” I
thought I understood. I thought I was prepared for the huge change coming. And
while I wasn’t unprepared, I really had no idea exactly how life-changing
becoming a parent would be. Now I try to explain to my friends who don’t have
children what exactly getting swept into parenthood felt like, and the best I
have come up with is this—I had my daughter and she was more wonderful than I
could have imagined, and the rest of my life fell into chaos. One of those
pieces of my life was my relationship with my husband. We look at each other
and marvel that we used to sit around on the weekend and lament that we did not
know what to do with ourselves. Now we would give anything to learn the secret
to freezing time. Now we try to hold on as life rushes by. Now I tell my
husband we need more time and he agrees but asks, “what time?”
Showing posts with label Children. Show all posts
Showing posts with label Children. Show all posts
Thursday, September 3, 2015
Friday, March 28, 2014
Parenthood, Trial or Tribulation? Part 2
Posted by
Amie
On New Year’s Day I
became a parent, sparking my curiosity in the research on parenting and
well-being and inspiring a four-part series on parenthood and happiness. This
is the second post. Check out the first post here.
Are parents happier
than non-parents? Researchers have generally set about trying to answer
this deceptively simple question in three ways:
Are people with children happier than those without children?
This is the most common approach to research on parenthood
and well-being. In these studies, researchers typically tackle large datasets
with thousands of adults, comparing the well-being of people with children to
people without children. Although the approach is straightforward, the results
are mixed with some studies finding parents are happier than non-parents and other
studies find the reverse.
How can these studies with such a basic design find opposite
results? One large problem with this approach is that little work is
done to find out who exactly is making up these groups of parents and
non-parents. Focusing on the non-parents, only 15% of adults do not have
children, making them a small comparison group. More importantly, their reasons
for doing so may differ greatly. Young adults may not have children when they
take part in the research, but plan to have children later. Older adults may
not have children because they were not able to do so, or they may have
consciously made the choice to not have children. Imagine comparing a married 48-year
old with three children to a married 48 year-old with no children who spent
years and hard earned dollars fighting infertility and wishing to be a parent?
Who do you think is happier? Now imagine that the non-parent comparison is a 48
year-old who loves to travel, lives all over the globe and chose not to have
children because they wouldn’t fit a globetrotting lifestyle. Who do you think
is happier? In one study, mothers were no happier than women who chose not to
have children, but were significantly happier than infertile women (Callan,
1987). Choice plays an important role on the other side of the table as well—some
people become parents by choice while others find themselves in the unexpected
position of being a parent when they hadn’t intended it. How might choice
affect happiness among these different groups?
Are people happier after they have children than they were before they
were parents?
Thursday, March 20, 2014
Parenthood: Trial or Tribulation?
Posted by
Amie
This is the first post in a
four-part series on parenthood and happiness.
In my short time as a parent I have experienced great joy,
love and gratitude as well as intense worry, and sometimes even sadness. Happily,
as I sit here typing up this post with my two and a half month old swaddled
next to me on the couch, eyeing me trustingly as she falls in and out of sleep,
I can say that the balance tends to weigh strongly on the side of joy. But in
those moments where I don’t have the luxury to type up this post because I’m
tending to a crying child, or changing a dirty diaper, I dream of the freedom
of my former life and the balance is just a bit more evenly weighted.
And the one thing I know with certainty is that I still have
no clue what exactly I’ve gotten myself into. While my days often stretch out in
front of me with the sameness that comes from having an infant with simple
needs, I also know that she is growing and changing at a rapid pace. Each week
we are in uncharted territory as she learns to smile, sit, and eventually walk,
talk and push back as she becomes her own independent person.
Thursday, June 13, 2013
The Daddy Chronicles II: Parenting Boosts Immune Function
Posted by
Unknown
I've been doing this whole parenting thing for almost three months now and it has been simultaneously gratifying, terrifying, exhausting, and fascinating. One thing I haven't been doing is sleeping, and because of this I have had a lot of time to read up on some neat research on new parents. Last time I wrote about how parenting reduces Testosterone in men. Today I blog about the relationship between parenting and immune function.
Can parenting boost the immune system?
Can parenting boost the immune system?
Wednesday, November 21, 2012
Your Thanksgiving Table - The Political Metaphor
Posted by
OlgaAntonenkoYoung
![]() |
Source |
This week, I'd like to follow up on Michael's post about the ways that our parents' attitudes shape our political ideals.
A recent study (Fraley, Griffin, Belsky & Roisman, 2012) has found that parents who tend to believe in authoritarian parenting raise kids more likely to become conservative. Those that have egalitarian parenting attitudes tend to have kids who become liberal.
As you fly home and think about your parents' strict or relaxed styles, I'd like to ask how it is that parents help shape our core values.
As you fly home and think about your parents' strict or relaxed styles, I'd like to ask how it is that parents help shape our core values.
Friday, September 7, 2012
Kids, school, and play: A look at what today’s youngest students are (and are not) doing in the classroom
Posted by
Kate Reilly Thorson
![]() |
source |
Wednesday, March 21, 2012
Neighborly Love: The Psychology of Mr. Rogers
Posted by
Juli
![]() |
Source |
Friday, February 3, 2012
Friday Fun: A few simple tricks for healthier eating
Posted by
Amie
![]() |
Who could resist? |
Here are a few of their scientifically-backed tips for making healthier food choices. Many of these tips have been put in place in lunchrooms as part of their “Smarter Lunchrooms Initiative,” but I think they can also be adapted for use at home, particularly if you are struggling with a child who has very particular food preferences.
Monday, October 31, 2011
What do Halloween and Social Psychology have in common? Deindividuation, of course
Posted by
Amie
![]() |
Cute trick-or-treaters or mahem-makers? |
As the children take to the streets tonight in search of a
trick-or-treat, you might be wondering the best way to protect your house from
some heavy candy-looting. In 1976, Ed Diener and his colleagues asked a similar question, though they were more
interested in the conditions that prompted trick-or-treaters to overindulge and
take more than they should. Halloween is a holiday which encourages people to
dress up in costumes and roam the streets in large groups - the perfect recipe
for deindividuation. Deindividuation
occurs when people’s own sense of individuality is diminished and can result in
antisocial behaviors. Diener used Halloween as an opportunity to research how
anonymity, group size, and feelings of responsibility influence people’s
willingness to steal extra candy and money.
The scene: Imagine
that you come up to a house with a table, on one side is a bowl full of
individually wrapped bite-sized candy bars, about 2 feet away on the other side
is a bowl full of pennies and nickels. Nearby is a decorative backdrop with a
peep hole that camouflages an unobtrusive observer. When you arrive at the
door, a woman you have never met greets you.
Wednesday, October 12, 2011
Want to become a wizard? Just read Harry Potter
Posted by
Anna Luerssen
![]() |
source |
I will never forget when the final installment of the Harry Potter series came out. Myself and a few of my closest friends from college, all big HP fans, were spending the weekend at my Mom’s house. Although I hadn’t seen these friends in 6 months, although there were a ton of activities to do in that region of upstate NY, although we were twenty five years old - we could not wait to see how J.K. Rowling was going to wrap up the series. The second we picked up the Deathly Hallows, we literally did not stop. We lounged around all day, moving from the sun chairs outside, to the porch, to our beds, and back. We ate, we drank, we read. We barely talked. Parmita and I, the most determined, read straight through the night – 759 pages in total. It was a marathon, and let me tell you, it was well worth it.
Though the power of a good book is undeniable even to the lightest of readers, researchers have discovered some unexpected benefits from an engaging narrative. For example, people tend to feel less lonely after reading a familiar narrative, and even seek out comforting books after experiences of social rejection (Derrick, Gabriel, & Hugenberg, 2009). Narratives have been found to help develop social skills – they teach us rules that govern social interactions and help us to cultivate empathy (e.g. Mar & Oatley, 2008). In an interesting study published recently in Psychological Science, Shira Gabriel and Ariana Young even found that we actually feel like, or become, the characters of the book, and that this assumption of the characters’ identities makes us feel happier and more satisfied with our own lives. Here's the study...
Wednesday, August 24, 2011
What you expect is what you get: The "Pygmalion Effect"
Posted by
Amie
![]() |
Source |
-Henry Ford
A couple of Fridays ago I posted a video about teacher who took her third grade class through an activity designed to help them learn about prejudice. When the students were told by their teacher that people with a certain eye color were smarter and better all around, they came to believe it and act in accordance. In the comments to this post, a reader noted that this video reminded him of the famous study by Rosenthal and Jacobson (1966) testing something they'd termed the “Pygmalion effect,” and so I thought I'd share that study with you today.
The Pygmalion effect? If you are not a fan of Eliza Doolittle and My Fair Lady, you might think this effect sounds like a medical condition that occurs after too much sun exposure (or is that just me?), but it’s not. What we’re talking about here is a simple case of self self-fulfilling prophecies (which Juli first wrote about here). Rosenthal and Jacobson were interested in the role of teacher expectancies in learning. What exactly does this mean? Imagine that a third grade teacher starts in the fall with a new class of students, a few of which had older sibling who passed through her class in previous years. She knows that those siblings were star students, and expects the younger siblings will also perform well. She might also talk with some of the second grade teachers who had had some of her students the previous year, and get all kinds of insider information about which students were top performers, and which straggled behind. Now let’s fast forward to the end of the year. Not surprisingly, the students whom the teacher had expected to do well met her expectations, and the stragglers continued to straggle behind. Did those star students perform well because they were smarter than the rest, as indicated by their siblings’ success and the reports of their second grade teachers? Or could it have been a much more sinister story - that they did so well simply because their teachers expected them to do well? This is exactly what Rosenthal and Jacobson wanted to find out.
Wednesday, July 27, 2011
Understanding Mean Girls
Posted by
Juli
![]() |
Regina from Mean Girls |
On the one hand, we're obsessed with them. Another Mean Girls sequel, Mean Moms, is forthcoming, and reality TV is replete with real-life mean girls of all kinds (for example, see these clips from The Real Housewives of New York, The Jersey Shore, and The Hills). Even after high school, we look to cultural alphas, such as high-profile celebrities, for advice on how to dress, what to eat (even if it's just lemon juice and cayenne pepper), and even how to surgically alter ourselves (see I want a famous face). There are apparently even elderly mean girls.
On the other hand, we resent their power and love to bring them down, which is probably why so many mean girl-centered movies (Heathers, Saved!, Sixteen Candles, etc) feature the demise of the queen bee and the triumph of the downtrodden, and why powerful female politicians are often torn apart by the media. See, for example, Carly Fiorina's infamous "mean girl moment" or Maureen Dowd's column on mean girl politicians. (Dowd herself was later accused of being "the ultimate mean girl").
Friday, June 3, 2011
Friday Fun? See a clip of the classic Bobo Doll experiment
Posted by
Anna Luerssen
![]() |
source |
In the classic “Bobo Doll” experiment child participants observed an adult interact aggressively with a plastic, blow-up doll. The adult hit the doll, kicked it, and even pummeled it with a mallet. Subsequently, each child was allowed to play with the doll. Children that observed the adult’s aggressive behavior toward the doll, behaved with similar forms of aggression. Children in a control condition, that did not view this aggressive modeling, did not play with the doll in an aggressive manner. This research suggests that children learn aggressive behavior, at least in part, by imitating parents, other adults, or peers behaving in this way.
Take a look at the following clip to see (somewhat disturbing) footage from the Bobo Doll experiment:
What do you think of this study? How else might aggressive behavior be learned?
Wednesday, May 11, 2011
One cookie or Two: Part 3
Posted by
Anna Luerssen
![]() |
source |
I’d like to conclude with some evidence on what helps or hurts a child’s chances of successfully waiting during the task. Remember, how long kids wait in childhood predicts important future functioning. If we want to help kids develop or cultivate delay of gratification ability in early life we can’t just say “wait for two cookies!” We need to give them tangible strategies to work with.
To identify which strategies help, researchers conducted experiments in which they modified tiny parts of the delay task and measured how these modifications impacted the amount of time children could wait. For a great review of these studies check out Mischel, Shoda, and Rodriguez (1989).
Wednesday, May 4, 2011
One cookie or two: Part 2
Posted by
Anna Luerssen
![]() |
source |
If you didn’t read the prior post, or can’t remember the task, take a look back before reading on. A quick reminder…a child waits A LONG TIME, ALONE, with NOTHING TO DO, in order to receive two treats rather than have just one treat immediately. The amount of time the child can wait serves as the measure of delay of gratification ability.
As I already mentioned, this task seems pretty ridiculous (albeit awesome to watch). Yet, I promised to describe “…all the cool and important outcomes behavior during this task predicts.” I will get to those outcomes today.
First let me provide a little historical context:
There are few psychology majors, and almost no psychology graduate students or professors who haven’t heard of Walter Mischel. In addition to his groundbreaking work on personality, Mischel created the delay of gratification task that I’ve described. Mischel’s original work on delay of gratification focused on understanding the features of the task that helped or hurt a child’s chances of waiting. For example, can children wait longer when the treats are sitting right in front of them or when they are covered? (What did he find? I’ll get to that in my next post)
In addition to this experimental research, Mischel was curious about whether individual differences in performance on the delay task relates to functioning in later life. Is this task capturing something about a child that is important and long-lasting? To answer this question, Mischel followed a group of participants overtime. These committed participants (and their parents) have completed various follow up assessments between the late 1960’s and early 1970’s (when they first completed the task) through to the present.
Wednesday, April 27, 2011
One cookie or two?
Posted by
Anna Luerssen
![]() |
source |
Young children and adults differ in many ways. The latter descriptions illustrate one important difference, however - the ability to delay gratification. One of the longest running, most famous, and to me, most interesting lines of research in the field of psychology has been conducted on delay of gratification. Today I will define this construct and explain how we quantify this ability in childhood. In future posts I’ll tell you about why delay of gratification matters and I’ll describe what helps or hurts us when we try to delay.
What is delay of gratification exactly?
This is the ability to forgo an immediate, but less desirable reward (e.g. Jersey Shore), in order to obtain a delayed, but more desirable reward later (e.g. getting into college). As I mentioned at the outset, with age we all get better at delay of gratification. Nevertheless, if we look within an age group, people differ in how well they can delay. I'm sure you can think of some friends who find it nearly impossible to study for a midterm, and others who spend their lives at the library in order to get into med school. Interestingly, individual differences in delay ability emerge in early childhood, and as I intimated, these differences in childhood predict important outcomes in later life.
Wednesday, April 13, 2011
No dessert until you eat your vegetables!
Posted by
Anna Luerssen
![]() |
source |
So what do parents, like mine, do when they are faced with a ding dong craving, pringles loving, child like me? If we look to classic psychology theory for an answer, the results are mixed. Research on behaviorism has shown that positive reinforcement – when a behavior is followed by the presence of rewards – makes the behavior happen more often. Applied to the green veggie quandary - when children eat the healthy parts of a meal, parents provide rewards (e.g. smiles, stickers, or simply cold hard cash). If the theory is correct this will increase the likelihood the child will eat more healthy foods in the future.
This idea has been challenged by another line of research, however. Self-determination theory suggests that providing extrinsic rewards actually undermines, or diminishes intrinsic motivation. Again, applied to the green veggie quandary, if parents reward children for eating the healthy parts of a meal, children will actually come to like those healthy parts less, and will only be motivated to eat them, in order to obtain the reward. Take away the reward, and the green veggies will stay on your child’s plate. So who is right?
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)