source |
source |
1. New pope. When
the 266th pope of the Catholic Church was elected this year, many praised him
for his commitment to interfaith dialogue. His support of cross-religious
interactions underscores his belief that communication between members of
different groups can help to reduce prejudice and conflict. Seeking to heal
rifts among people due to religious differences or prior conflicts, the pope himself
has sought out personal relationships with many religious leaders across the
globe. He hopes that by encouraging his followers to establish similar
interfaith relationships, current tensions can be quelled and prejudice
alleviated.
source |
Research on the intergroup contact hypothesis tells us that
the pope’s strategy is likely a good one. The basic prediction of the
hypothesis is that contact between people of different groups will usually
reduce prejudice. Forming relationships with members of another group can help people
learn more about a group, experience less anxiety about interacting with the group,
and feel more empathy for that group. All three of these outcomes can then diminish
prejudice. Some research has even shown that merely having a friend who
interacts with someone from another group can reduce prejudice (the extended
contact hypothesis). While it’s not always the case that intergroup contact
yields less prejudice, the pope does seem to be taking the right approach for
prejudice reduction by continuing to support interfaith dialogue.
source |
2. Boston marathon
attacks. It’s no secret that we turn to others in times of stress or that
collective traumas change us. When bombs exploded near the finish line of the
Boston marathon in April, Bostonians reached out to support one another. People
across the city provided resources and emotional support to strangers, and
throughout the nation, people proclaimed their solidarity with Boston. To cope
with the vulnerability felt after the attack, people sought to process their
thoughts surrounding the event with others: face-to-face with another person,
within support groups, and through social media.
source |
After traumas, research has revealed that people shift in
the types of social interactions they prefer. Work conducted after 9/11 showed
that people gradually engaged in more one-on-one, in-person interactions across
the ten days after the attacks and participated in fewer group interactions and
phone conversations. The research also suggested that increases in these in-person interactions facilitated greater psychological adjustment post-9/11. Not
only can stressful events change the types of interactions we seek, but it can
also change the way we perceive our social environments – for the better. One
study showed that 58% of study participants believed there were social benefits
of 9/11, such as more prosocial behavior and political involvement in their
communities. Hopefully there were some such benefits of the horrific and tragic
bombings in Boston.
source |
source |
Hopefully, adding a little psychological research to some of
the top news stories of 2013 helped you see them from a slightly different
perspective. Here’s to 2014 and the top news stories it brings!
Let us know what you
think about the psychology within these news stories and what else you think
psychology can add to them!
References:
McCullough ME, Rachal KC, Sandage SJ, Worthington EL Jr, Brown SW, & Hight TL (1998). Interpersonal forgiving in close relationships: II. Theoretical elaboration and measurement. Journal of personality and social psychology, 75 (6), 1586-603 PMID: 9914668
Mehl MR, & Pennebaker JW (2003). The social dynamics of a cultural upheaval: social interactions surrounding September 11, 2001. Psychological science, 14 (6), 579-85 PMID: 14629689
Pettigrew, T.F., & Tropp, L. R. (2008). How does intergroup contact reduce prejudice? Meta-analytic tests of three mediators European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 922-934
Poulin MJ, Silver RC, Gil-Rivas V, Holman EA, & McIntosh DN (2009). Finding social benefits after a collective trauma: perceiving societal changes and well-being following 9/11. Journal of traumatic stress, 22 (2), 81-90 PMID: 19283834
Witvliet CV, Ludwig TE, & Vander Laan KL (2001). Granting forgiveness or harboring grudges: implications for emotion, physiology, and health. Psychological science, 12 (2), 117-23 PMID: 11340919
Wright, S. C., Aron, A., McLaughlin-Volpe, T., & Ropp, S. A. (1997). The extended contact effect: Knowledge of cross-group friendships and prejudice Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 73-90
I'm surprised by the findings that link the trauma of 9/11 to a lower share of group interactions vs. one-on-one interactions. I would guess (and wonder) that (if) the total # of group interactions and one-on-one interactions both increased, but that one-on-ones increased more.
ReplyDeleteInsightful again. WDM
ReplyDelete