If
you visit this blog occasionally (or follow me on twitter, because of course!)
then you know that social psychology has come under criticism recently for its
lack of integrity in research methods and the complete absence of exact
replication. The criticism has been strongest with respect to a subfield in
social psychology known as social
priming. Social priming refers to a now classic psychological phenomenon
where the activation of one social concept in memory can elicit changes in
behavior, physiology, or self-reports of a related social concept without
conscious awareness. Social priming has been used to explain why reading words
related to elderly concepts (e.g., Florida, retire) can lead you to walk slower
(although, this particular social priming finding does not replicate across different
laboratories).
The
criticism about social priming stems from (a) a growing number of high profile replication attempts of classic findings that failed, (b) the steady stream of fanciful/unbelievable
social priming effects that appear in glamour psychology journals (e.g., messyrooms make people more creative), and (c)
the inappropriate reactions of prominent social priming researchers to this
scrutiny (e.g., criticizing the scientific integrity of replication attempts and the journals that publish those attempts). It’s been like watching a car
wreck in super slow motion!
I
have taken the criticism directed at social psychology, and social priming
specifically, very personally over the last couple of years. This is because I
consider myself (and still do) a social priming researcher. One of my graduate
advisers is Serena Chen. She was trained at New York University, and one of her
advisers there was John Bargh—the poster child of social priming. Bargh is
basically an academic grandparent of mine. So, to see him attacked and then react
to criticisms and failed replications so inappropriately is a little
disconcerting for me and the field more broadly.
What
is worse is that much of the criticism directed at social priming research is
warranted. Experiments are often designed without proper control conditions, manipulations
aren’t checked for conscious participant expectations, samples are way too
small, articles frequently over-claim the practical impact of the findings, and
almost no one conducts (or reports/publishes) direct replications. This might
not be so bad if it didn’t cause any harm, but it does. It harms the field of
social psychology because shoddy methods in social priming give the world the
impression that all of social psychology (psychology even) lacks methodological
rigor. It harms new graduate students because there is no clear road map to
lead students to effects that are robust and away from effects that won’t
replicate (we dealt with a ton of this in my graduate school lab meetings, many
a career was delayed). It harms the researchers who are actually conducting
good social priming research: The impression is that if you are conducting
social priming research, then ipso facto, your findings won’t replicate.
On
this latter point, perhaps you are one of the people who thinks that ALL social
priming research is poorly conducted. Well, you’re wrong! There is good,
replicable, social priming research out there. I think this is a nice time to
draw attention to a finding that has been directly replicated. The finding is: The Transference Effect.
First,
let me define transference in social priming terms. Transference occurs when a
new acquaintance resembles an individual’s significant other, whom they hold a
representation of in memory. When the individual meets this new acquaintance,
the resemblance leads the individual to misremember
information about the acquaintance in ways that are consistent with the
significant other AND to evaluate the
acquaintance as the significant other is evaluated. Thus, if my spouse meets a
new acquaintance who incidentally has some of my characteristics, transference
will occur, and she will automatically assume that the acquaintance likes
basketball, lasagna, and Game of Thrones, and also will evaluate the
acquaintance more positively than if he hadn’t had some of my characteristics
(we are assuming in this example that my spouse evaluates me positively!).
In
classic transference research, participants sit next door to a (presumed) real
person and read about either the personal characteristics of that person, manipulated
either to resemble their own significant other or someone else’s significant
other. This procedure ensures that even the control description of the person
next door has features of a real/living/breathing person and so only the significant-other-resemblance
can be used to explain the transference phenomenon. As well, transference
experiments use strict funnel debriefing procedures to make sure that
participants do not consciously connect the new acquaintance to their
significant other (participants describe their significant other in an
unrelated pre-test weeks prior). With these methods, transference research gets
close to providing evidence that the association between the primed construct
(i.e., the significant-other representation) and the outcome (i.e., memory and
affect) occurs outside of conscious awareness.
The
main reason I chose to highlight transference research is that it also has several
examples of direct replication. This is a real rarity in contemporary social psychology! The original memory effect was demonstrated in
1990 in a paper by Susan Andersen and Steve Cole. The effect has since been directly
replicated in 1994 (Andersen & Baum, 1994), 2006 (Brumbaugh & Fraley,
2006), 2009 (Kraus & Chen, 2009), 2010 (Kraus, Chen, Lee, & Straus,
2010), and there was a failed direct replication in 1996 (Hinkley &
Andersen, 1996). Of course, the samples are small so the methods aren’t perfect.
In
terms of the evaluation effect of transference, this effect was demonstrated first
in 1994 by Alana Baum and Susan Andersen (Andersen & Baum, 1994). This
effect has been directly replicated in 1999 (Baum & Andersen, 1999), 2009
(Kraus & Chen, 2009), and there was one successful and one failed direct
replication in 2010 (Kraus et al., 2010). Again, smaller samples, but still a
step in the right direction!
Social
priming researchers are faced with the public perception that our field lacks
methodological rigor. In the past, the dominant reaction has been to simply
downplay these concerns and assure people that our findings replicate. This is
the wrong strategy! Instead, we should place greater emphasis on direct
replication in our own research and make an effort to work directly with other
researchers who are interested in the direct replication of these findings. This
is the way forward—the way to reveal the social priming research that works!
References
Andersen, S. M., & Baum, A. (1994). Transference in Interpersonal Relations: Inferences and Affect Based on Significant‐Other Representations. Journal of personality, 62(4), 459-497.
Andersen, S. M., & Cole, S. W. (1990). " Do I know you?": The role of significant others in general social perception. Journal of personality and social psychology, 59(3), 384.
Baum, A., & Andersen, S. M. (1999). Interpersonal roles in transference: Transient mood effects under the condition of significant-other resemblance.Social cognition, 17(2), 161-185.
Brumbaugh, C. C., & Fraley, R. C. (2006). Transference and attachment: How do attachment patterns get carried forward from one relationship to the next?.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(4), 552-560.
Hinkley, K., & Andersen, S. M. (1996). The working self-concept in transference: significant-other activation and self change. Journal of personality and social psychology, 71(6), 1279.
Kraus, M. W., & Chen, S. (2009). Striving to be known by significant others: Automatic activation of self-verification goals in relationship contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 58.
Kraus, M. W., Chen, S., Lee, V. A., & Straus, L. D. (2010). Transference occurs across group boundaries. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,46(6), 1067-1073.
No comments:
Post a Comment