"Laughter
and tears are both responses to frustration and exhaustion. I myself prefer to
laugh, since there is less cleaning up to do afterward."
-Kurt
Vonnegut
source |
Potentially
life altering events such as the dissolution of a long-term romantic
relationship, the birth of a child, the death of a loved one, the loss of a
job, moving to a new city (or just across town, for that matter), or any other such
experiences are major causes of stress. Recent research in social and
personality psychology has demonstrated that we can adopt several strategies to
help us decrease the negative emotions surrounding these events (Gross and
Thompson, 2007). Among the most adaptive responses to stressors is cognitive reappraisal. In other words, one
way to decrease the stress of a negative event is to think about it in a
different way, in order to consciously change the event’s meaning, and thereby,
lesson its emotional impact.
For instance, the religious among us may
reinterpret the passing of a loved one as representing a transition to a better
place and as being part of a higher power’s (i.e., God) grand plan. The less
religious among us may view it as an opportunity for the loved one to no longer
have to deal with the annoyances of daily life or to no longer suffer the pain
and discomfort associated with an illness. Viewing events in these more
positive ways makes us feel much better than if we simply focus on the negative
aspects of the event.
Cognitive
reappraisal stands in contrast to the less adaptive strategy of suppression, in which we fully
experience negative emotions but do not display any behavioral signs. For
instance, some of us try to show strength rather than crying when we are sad.
Unfortunately, when using suppression, we are so focused on not showing any
signs of our discomfort that it actually causes us more rather than less
distress.
Can humor mend a broken heart? (source) |
Recently, following the end of a
long-term romantic relationship, family and friends seemingly expected me to be
depressed and constantly in tears. “Aren’t you sad?,” “I’m so sorry,” “That
sucks,” and “Aren’t you lonely?” were among the most frequent phrases and
questions I heard from loved ones in the post-breakup months. It was probably
extremely confusing to them when I would laugh a bit or smile and say, “it’s ok,”
“I’m fine,” or “I’m happier than I’ve
ever been,” in response. Surely they assumed I was suppressing my sadness. Was
I? How was I able to cope with my new found identity as a bachelor? Some of you
may already be thinking reappraisal—a young, single man living in the Bay Area (one
of the hippest and most beautiful areas in the country) sounds amazing. Further,
it could be seen as a fun opportunity to search for someone who would
appreciate me not just despite, but because of my flaws. True, but these
reappraisals didn’t fully explain my mirthful attitude…
What about humor and laughing? Can it be
used to regulate our emotions in responses to stress? Folk wisdom says laughter
is the best medicine and that humor soothes the soul. Indeed, while looking for
media to use for a research project, I came across the stand-up comedy of
Richard Pryor (yes, this was for a scientific pursuit, I promise!). Infamous
for his raunchy, ground-breaking routines about race relations and sex, it came
as a pleasant surprise when I heard his take on romantic relationships and heartbreak.
In fact, watching Pryor’s exaggerated imitation of a heartbroken man
accidentally wandering into the street and getting hit by a bus made me laugh
so hard that I immediately sought out other comedic routines about
relationships (Louis C. K., Kevin Hart, and the late, great, Patrice O’Neal are
highly recommended!). Through a little
introspection, I realized that I did still feel a little sad about the breakup,
but I also felt much more positive than I had prior to watching these
comedians. One question remained: why did I have these mixed emotions?
Philosophers such as Plato have posited that humor involves a mixture of both
pleasure and pain, but what does psychological research have to say about the
topic?
McGraw and Warren (2010) have reported a
set of studies that appear to agree with philosophers and lay theories.
According to their benign violation hypothesis, 3 conditions are necessary and
sufficient for eliciting humor. First, the humor must be seen as a violation
(including apparent threats, violation of norms, or involve taboo content).
Second, this violation must be seen as benign (through occurring in contexts
that are perceived as safe, playful, or non-serious). Third, both of these
appraisals must occur simultaneously.
source |
In their studies, McGraw and Warren
(2010) randomly assigned participants to either an experimental or a control condition.
In the experimental condition, participants read several vignettes where people
engaged in acts that could be interpreted as benign moral violations in which
no harm is done (for example, a man rubs his bare genitals on his pet kitten
and the kitten purrs and appears to enjoy the contact). In the control
condition the moral violations cause harm (the contact causes the kitten
discomfort). After reading each passage, participants reported on the emotions
they were currently experiencing. Importantly, participants who read the
experimental vignettes were significantly more likely to hold mixed emotions and
be simultaneously disgusted and amused by the behavior compared to those who
read the control vignettes. Those who read the control vignettes were only
disgusted by the behavior. So, although humor doesn’t decrease negative
emotions, it does increase and focus one on positive emotions.
The increase in positive emotions received
after exposure to humor may bestow extra cognitive resources necessary to deal
with any lingering negative emotions associated with life’s stressors. It may
also provide a sense of belonging, which one may need following rejection
(after all, laughter is more common in the presence of others and we have to
have some shared knowledge in order to understand a joke). This may shed light
on why the antics of Pryor, C.K., Hart, and O’Neal allowed me to overcome a
difficult breakup. With their help, I was able to reappraise the situation,
deal with any negative feelings, feel much more positive, and see that the end
of a relationship is something that others have gone through and survived—I wasn’t
alone! For me, laughter was indeed the best medicine.
Rather than only shedding tears and
ruminating on our negative emotions, a more positive way to deal with stress
may be to laugh and try to find humor in the situation—as Vonnegut argues, the
emotional cleanup can be much easier if we do!
James
Telesford is currently a Ph.D. candidate in the social-personality psychology
program at UC Berkeley. His broad research interests consist of 1) the effects
of using racial humor on intergroup relations and 2) the impact of Black racial
identity on several outcomes, including academic achievement and psychological
well-being.
Further
Reading:
Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual
foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook
of Emotion Regulation, (pp. 3-24). New York: Guilford Press.McGraw AP, & Warren C (2010). Benign violations: making immoral behavior funny. Psychological science, 21 (8), 1141-9 PMID: 20587696
Nice post! It's very helpful and enjoyable to read. :D
ReplyDeletebenign moral violations in which no harm is done (for example, a man rubs his bare genitals on his pet kitten and the kitten purrs and appears to enjoy the contact). Buy now
ReplyDelete