I took a course in sociology my first
year as an undergraduate at UC Berkeley. The course was an introduction to
sociology taught by professor and social activist, Harry Edwards. The course
blew me away because it felt so viscerally real. Professor Edwards would talk
about social class, race, and gender in America and students would chime in
about their own experiences that brought these big social constructs to life.
What I learned in Professor Edwards’ class resembled nothing we had discussed
in my high school history classes—I grew up in a politically conservative
suburb in San Diego, and we didn’t have much ideological diversity in our
discussions of law and society. Sociology, and social sciences more broadly,
really spoke to me.
I majored in psychology and sociology
upon graduation from Berkeley. This education eventually led to my own
empirical research program on social class with an advisor, Dacher Keltner,
willing to and interested in studying a big construct with hundreds of years of
theory behind it, but little empirical examination (in psychology). More
broadly though, I think I just wanted to find a career where I would need to
think like I did as an undergraduate. There aren’t a lot of places outside the
social sciences where a person can feel safe and secure about critically
discussing left-leaning social justice issues.
But perhaps there are costs to having
these ideologically safe, but segregated, spaces in an empirical discipline? A
couple of weeks ago Behavioral Brain Sciences published a thought-provokingpiece about the lack of ideological diversity in the social sciences. This lack
of diversity of viewpoint in the social sciences threatens the validity of (some)
research according to the authors—because ideologically similar individuals
share blind spots.
Wow, that’s potentially a big problem
without an easy solution! After reading the article and the commentary, and
discussing the topic at length with the pigee group, I have come to some
observations. These observations aren’t particularly insightful or
ground-breaking—many were covered in the article and commentary—but I’ll share
them anyway (in bullet point format).
· My ideological views make me the researcher I am. Without my
own subjective way of seeing the world, I would not have the motivation or
interest to conduct research on the topics I do and in the way I do. Ideology
must therefore be a necessary component of social science. It is important
then, to acknowledge that ideological subjectivity is a part of social science
and that balancing that ideology might not be possible for some of the topics
we study. For instance, maybe your political views indicate that social class
doesn’t exist in America. In that case, you could either write an article
claiming that, or rather, you would probably more likely just study something
else that actually does exist.
· A lack of ideological diversity is a problem, but perhaps
the causal order presented in the article is reversed: Liberals don’t choose
social psychology, instead, social psychology—with its focus on the power of
situational forces to shape people’s thoughts and behaviors—enhances or
re-affirms liberal ideology in its practitioners. I’ve shifted to be more and
more (and more) liberal-leaning the more I’ve studied social psychology (n = 1,
p < .05).
· Can’t we simply evaluate research based on its rigor, while
leaving our own ideological viewpoints at the door? Hey, we’re all scientists
here! It should be challenging, but possible to say something like the
following as a reviewer of another scholar’s ideologically aligned research:
“This makes a lot of sense, but is the evidence strong or weak?”
· Ending ideological discrimination seems like a no-brainer
because eliminating discriminatory practices is a liberal value and
social-personality psychology is full of political liberals ;).
· There are many scientific disciplines with differing
ideological makeup, many of the disciplines study the same research questions
as social-personality psychology, but using their own unique methods. For
instance, there are many economists who study economic inequality (just like
there are psychologists who study it), and economists tend to be higher in
libertarianism. Perhaps the easiest solution to increasing ideological diversity
might be to require greater interdisciplinary scholarship—that is, researchers
should be asked to integrate what economists and psychologists say about
economic inequality in their own empirical work.
· Liberal ideology is not a monolithic ideological platform.
There are many discussion topics that a group of two or three self-identified
liberal psychologists would end in disagreeable arguments. Certainly some
ideological views are not well represented, but perhaps there is enough
ideological diversity to solve many of the problems listed in the article.
· Social science fields are some of the only places where
liberal ideology is protected and fostered. If we develop ideological enclaves
in the social sciences, it’s not likely that these enclaves extend to the
grocery store, to public schools attended by our children, to Thanksgiving
dinner with family, or to cultural experiences (e.g., “I’d like to sit in the
liberal section of the stadium please.”). In fact, I would wager that social
scientists must contend with competing ideologies all the time in these public
spaces. Or perhaps not.
No comments:
Post a Comment